|
No genetic cause has as yet been proved for any transsexual manifestation. In a few
rare cases of the Klinefelter syndrome, being complicated by transsexualism (or
vice versa), the usual genetic fault was found, the patients showing 47 chromosomes
(instead of the normal 46), with a chromosomal constellation of XXY instead of XY. At the
same time, there were the usual clinical findings (see Chapters II and III). All
transsexual patients without complicating disorders so far reported showed a normal
chromosomal sex.
A theory such as that would indeed explain much better than psychological "conditioning" the astonishing depth and the intensity with which a transsexual identifies with the opposite sex. Incidentally, it would also explain the resistance to treatment. Per Anchersen, head of the department of psychiatry at the City Hospital of Oslo, Norway, briefly discusses etiology in his paper "Problems of Transvestism." [5] He has himself examined six transsexuals to whom, however, he refers as "genuine transvestites," using Hamburger's terminology. He found no symptoms of "primary mental disease." He feels that bisexuality, some forms of homosexuality, and transvestism are "different stages along a line of sexual deviation." Two of the transvestites were a monozygotic pair of twins. "From their early childhood they behaved like girls." After their sixteenth year, they "have been working separated in kitchens." . . . Both "stressed their aversions to homosexual men . . . " one of the twins urgently wished to change his sex, the other preferred to wait and see how his brother was going on . . . . "Intellectually both were retarded with an I.Q. of 74." This seems so far the only case of identical twins among transsexuals reported in the medical literature. 1 have seen none among my own patients. Anchersen quotes Kallmann from his studies of homosexual twins as follows: "In 40 monozygotic pairs of twins there was not only a complete concordance concerning the homosexuality as such. Even the development and the performance of the sexual activity were quite identical. . . . In many cases, the twins lived separated from their early childhood." [6] Albert Ellis, the noted psychologist, disagrees and says:
Per Anchersen himself concludes: "the homosexuality manifested in the genuine
transvestite seems to belong to the constitutional form." A possible endocrine cause of transsexualism has been investigated in a few cases with great thoroughness. Beyond a few suspicious findings, no definite proof has as yet been found. It may or may not have an endocrine significance that among my 152 male transsexuals, nearly 40 per cent were found to have more or less distinct signs of a degree of sexual underdevelopment (hypogonadism), as was mentioned previously. In such a condition, the pituitary as well as the gonads may be at fault with, of course, an inborn reason behind it. A few years ago the American psychiatrist, Robert J. Stoller, and his collaborators [8] reported the case of an evidently transsexual man who had a typically feminine body build with feminine hair distribution, but with testes and a normal penis and without internal female organs as revealed through laparotomy. Nevertheless, there was "evidence of continuing estrogen influence from a source which has not been determined . . . The microscopic examination of testicular tissue has failed to reveal estrogenproducing cells." Schwabe and his collaborators,[9] however, reported shortly afterward that in another, probably transsexual male, large amounts of estrogen (more than double the normal) were found in the testes. The hormone-producing Leydig cells were held responsible for this estrogen production. More investigations have been made along these lines with negative or doubtful results and still more are in progress. It must not be forgotten that transsexual patients are not too frequent and that reliable scientific studies can be made only where the necessary facilities exist, that is to say, through hospitals, laboratories, and research institutes. In recent years, evidence has accumulated that hormone medication during pregnancy can have serious consequences for the newborn. If the mother was given testosterone or progesterone for any length of time during her pregnancy (usually to prevent abortion), genital deformities of the newborn may result and have resulted if the genetic sex of the baby was female. Pseudohermaphroditism was the consequence. Here, a thought is bound to occur: What if the fetus is a male? It is normally under the influence of the mother's female hormone (her estrogen) for nine months. Could that, under certain circumstances, interfere with the development so that the maleness of the newborn is repressed and a too feminine or underdeveloped infant is born? [10] Maybe the mother's progesterone and her small amounts of testosterone could, and probably normally do, act as a "brake," neutralizing the estrogen, or a metabolic conversion takes place automatically somewhere in the body. (Liver?) But maybe this does not always happen. Maybe an especially sensitive "sex center" in that small brain somehow becomes impaired in its development, either in its structure or in its chemistry, by the maternal estrogen. Could that explain why there are so many more male transsexuals, transvestites, and homosexuals than female? A child's brain is different from an adult's. The brain waves of an encephalogram do not begin to show an adult pattern until the child is four years old or even older.[11] What may be harmless to an adult may be detrimental to the young child. For instance, estrogen. The female transsexual naturally would need a different explanation, if the mother's endocrine status during pregnancy is being considered. Could an abnormal conversion of estrogen into testosterone take place so that a disturbed chemical mechanism underlies the biological one? Speculation may be allowed in an area that is still as obscure as that of gender disorientation from earliest childhood on. A new line of observation and investigation may have to be opened up. Another interesting observation, neither genetic nor endocrine, but nevertheless organic, was made some years ago by three American public health physicians, Drs. E. G. Williams, J. D. Reichard, and M. Pescor.[12] It concerned the reaction of the nervous system to Prostigmin, a rather powerful drug that acts directly on the nerves. Normal males and females react alike. So do homosexual males. The drug, however, had no affect at all on the nerves of "feminine men." According to the authors, this may indicate a possible inborn physical trait having to do with an enzyme that takes part in the chemical reaction through which nerves stimulate muscular action. To the best of my knowledge, these experiments have not been repeated as yet and therefore no confirmation or elaboration of the observation is available. In the light of the following paragraphs, however, they seem to gain particular significance. Related to the genetic as well as the endocrine possibilities of etiology is a most recent one, coming from Williarn C. Young [13] and his group at the Oregon Regional Primate Research Center. It may be termed the neural or cerebroneural one. The neural structures and brain centers are the "target," that is to say, receiving organs for hormonal influences. Their genetic quality can decide how these hormones may affect them. The Oregon group, working largely with monkeys, point to the "mechanism of hormonal action in organizing the tissues of the central nervous system." They say, "Evidence has accumulated indicating that the gonadal hormones have a broad role in the determination of (sex) behavior" through their "differentiation or organization of neural tissues." And so, after fifty years and more, the fundamental experiments of Eugen Steinach [14] of Vienna, who masculinized castrated females by implanting testicles and feminized castrated males through ovarian implants (and later female hormone injections) have found a modern substantiation, explanation, and elaboration. Recent brain research has likewise revealed possible pertinent facts. A frontal lobotomy, for instance, severing connections between the cerebral cortex and certain parts of deeper, more primitive centers (of the limbic system) sometimes results in bizarre and uninhibited sexual behavior. On other occasions, in clinical work, for instance, the lobotomy eliminated such behavior. With the help of exceedingly fine electrodes inserted into the brain structure, response to stimulation could be tested. Moving these electrodes only a fraction of a millimeter, either fear or anger or sexual excitation would be elicited. The possibility of other organic causes may be thought of, such as early encephalitic infections or brain injuries at birth, but no evidence along such lines has as yet been found. However, a report recently came from Dr. Roger A. Gorsky of the Brain Research Institute of the University of California at Los Angeles that may prove to be of greatest importance. Dr. Gorsky, as reported in Science Newsletter [15] found that at least a portion of the brain, known as the hypothalamus, is inherently feminine. "Unless there is testicular tissue secreting testosterone during this period of development to organize this portion of the brain along masculine lines, it remains forever feminine." Since the hypothalamus has much to do with the regulation of the pituitary function, secondary endocrine anomalies could well occur. A psychic trauma, too, seems to have produced a period of transvestism. A case has been
reported to me of a man who had never previously been a transvestite; but after the sudden
death of his father, he turned to cross-dressing. After a few months, the desire
disappeared as suddenly as it had started. It seems likely, however, that the tendency
toward TVism existed as a latent or suppressed condition and the psychic shock merely
"triggered" it into reality. Such a psychic mechanism may be operative to
explain occasional actions of the "pseudo-transvestite" (Type I in S.O.S.). The possible psychological causes of transsexualism have received much more attention and also more endorsement than those that could be called "organic" (at least in the American literature). Among those causes, the phenomenon of imprinting should be mentioned first. I M P R I N T I N G Green and Money [17] of Johns Hopkins School of Medicine have this to say:
Green and Money then continue to draw these conclusions:
The authors very clearly indicate here the possibility of an inherited predisposition for imprinting and naturally for its consequences. They found among their patients an "infrequency of forceful parental dominance in the household" and also "the relatively fragile body build of many of these boys." This again combines imprinting, a psychological factor, with body build, a physical attribute. The difficulty of proving (not only assuming), imprinting lies in the fact that parents may not remember the details in their households during the very early lives of their children and the patients themselves can hardly help. But their incongruous gender role is already recognizable when they are still very small.[18] C H I L D H O O D C O N D I T I O N I N G Literally, or in substance, here are statements that were made to me by transsexual patients:
Many similar early histories of transsexuals as well as of transvestites could be gleaned from the literature and certainly from my private correspondence. (See also the case histories in R. E. L. Masters' chapter). Lukianowicz [19] in his comprehensive survey not only relates some of his own observations, but quotes at length those of other authors. The possibility should, however, not be overlooked, that some of these patients may prefer such explanation to an inborn one and therefore allow a wish to be the father of their thought. Buchner found among his 262 TVs (with a small but unknown percentage of TSs) the following:
These figures are based on the first eighteen years of life. Taking a more vital earlier period, conditioning may be much more important. "Janet Thompson" says: "It seems evident to me that the inception of TVism falls in the one- to five-year-old period of the child' s life as a result of faulty, incomplete, or distorted sex identification." There can be no doubt that unfavorable early childhood experiences can constitute truly corrosive emotional traumata. That can neither be denied nor minimized. Yet, for the sake of scientific objectivity, it should be repeated here that many similar histories from the first few years of life can be elicited from persons who grew into perfectly normal adulthood later on. Those histories rarely become known, simply because "normal," well-adjusted men and women do not go to psychologists as a rule and one would have to look among one's friends and acquaintances for examples. With a little effort, they would be readily available. We all know men who lost their fathers at an early age, devoted their lives for years and years to their mothers, and by all psychoanalytic theories should have become homosexuals, transvestites, or transsexuals. But they did not. They had girl friends off and on and married as soon as the mother had passed away. It seems to me that conditioning cannot be the whole story. Unless there is a constitutional weakness, conditioning won't "take." Around the turn of the century, it was widely customary to raise boys almost the same as girls. They kept their long curls and wore dresses till they were five or six, that is to say, during rather critical years. Winston Churchill was one of those children, according to early pictures of him. Were there more transvestites, and the like then than there are now? Certainly not. A question of cause and effect should be raised in this connection. Could it not be that a constitutionally rather feminine-looking boy "conditioned" his parents so that they were inclined to forget about the tiny sex organs and reared him as a girl until it was time to send him to school? Especially if they had hoped for a girl? " He always looked and behaved more like a girl than a boy," is the explanation that parents gave to me to justify their errors. Whenever "conditioning" went against a healthy boy's true nature, no harm was done. As soon as he was old enough, he would rebel against the girl's dresses, because he wanted to be like all the other boys. But when the false upbringing harmonized with a constitution of a high feminine component, then it was a different story. Then the ground could have been laid for a future sex and gender disharmony. In one case that I observed some years ago, a kind of reverse situation actually seemed to exist. The parents were very glad to have the boy that was born to them, but at the age of three or four the child became very unhappy and difficult and wanted to be dressed and treated "like all other girls." The parents and two older sisters fought for a son and a brother, but finally had to give in to keep the peace. They allowed the little boy to wear girl's dresses, but insisted on a regular boy's haircut. These constituted the most distressing moments in the boy's life. Finally, he had to go to school as a boy and grew up into an extremely feminine-looking transsexual and transvestite. Desperately clamoring to have a conversion operation performed, he was studied by two groups of psychiatrists at the University of ______. One group recommended the operation as the only way to protect the patient's sanity. The other group advised against it, because they considered it unlikely that it would solve the underlying psychological problems. Psychotherapy was attempted for a short time, but failed, perhaps owing to the patient's lack of cooperation. With financial help from the mother who was sympathetic toward a change, the patient at last went abroad and succeeded in realizing his life's ambition. The operation was performed, but only partially so. For unknown reasons, no vagina was constructed at that time. In spite of that defect, the patient seemed more contented and emotionally better balanced when I saw him several weeks after the operation: better, at least, than on a visit to my office two years prior. He went to work as a woman, but the desire to become more complete never left him. After another two years, he returned to Europe for his vaginal plastic. The last I heard was that the operation was not successful because a fistula formed between the artificial vagina and the rectum. A further operation, however, may in the meantime have corrected this condition. In any event, this case seems to prove that an unknown constitutional factor was at the bottom of the gender disorientation and that "conditioning" evidently played no part in this instance. As reported in a lecture at the Albert Einstein College of Medicine (Jacobi Hospital) in April 1964, in 122 cases of male transsexualism among my own patients, conditioning in childhood could be shown in twenty-five cases (20 per cent). To this figure we may possibly add thirty-two doubtful cases (26 per cent). In 64 cases (56 per cent), no evidence of conditioning could be found. (In three cases, the early history was unknown.) C. V. Prince, editor of Transvestia, also investigated the probable frequency of conditioning by sending out questionnaires to 166 known transvestites, containing an unknown but small number of transsexuals. Conditioning was reported in "fewer than 20 per cent," according to Prince, who published his findings in Sexology Magazine.[20] More recently, H. Taylor Buchner, from the Survey Research Center of the University of California, who had sent out a quesionnaire to 262 subscribers of Transvestia (mostly transvestites) reported the following data as far as the problem of conditioning is concerned:
Summarizing my impression, I would like to repeat here what I said in my first lecture on the subject more than ten years ago:
Footnotes [1] Stourzh-Anderle, Helene, Sexuelle Konstitution, 1955, Verlag f. Medizinische Wissenschaften, Wien-Bonn. [2] Schlegel, W. S., Die Sexualinstinkte des Menschen, Ruetten und Loening Verlag, Hamburg, 1962. [3] 1964, Vol. 8, pp. 157-159. Pergamon Press, Ltd. [4] J. Urology, Vol. 91, No. 4, April 1964. [5] Ancherson, Per, "Problems of Transvestism," Acta Psych. et Neurol. Scandin.,Suppl. 106, 1956, p. 249. [6] Kallmann, F. J., Amer. J. Human Genetics, 1952, IV, pp. 136-146. [7] Ellis, Albert, Advances in Sex Research, Hoeber Medical Division, Harper and Row, 1963, p. 164. [8] Stoller, R. J., Garfinkel, H., and Rosen, A. C., "Passing and the Maintenance of Sexual Identification in an Intersexed Patient." A.M.A. Archives General Psychiatry, Vol. 2, April 1960, pp. 379-384. [9] Schwabe, A. D., et al., Pubertal Feminization in a Genetic Male with Testicular Atrophy and Normal Urinary Gonadotropin," J. Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, Vol. 22, August 1962, pp. 839-845. [10] A feminizing influence of estrogen has been demonstrated when a mother, soon after her confinement, began taking birth control pills (which contain estrogen) and at the same time, breast-fed her baby. Enough estrogen entered her milk to produce enlargement of the baby's breasts. [11] Rudolf Engel, J. Lancet, Vol. 81, 1961, p. 523. [12] "Homosexuality: A Biological Anomaly," J. Nervous & Mental Diseases, Vol. 99, No. 65, January 1944. [13] Young, William C., Goy, Robert W., Phoenix, Charles H., "Hormones and Sexual Behavior," Science, Vol. 143, No. 3603, January 17, 1964. [14] Steinach, Eugen, Sex and Life, Viking Press, New York, 1940. [15] Science Newsletter, August 28, 1965. [16] Lorenz, K. Z., King Solomons Ring. New Light on Animal Ways. The Thomas Y. Crowell Co., New York, 1952. [17] Green, R., and Money, J., "Effeminacy in Prepubertal Boys. Summary of 11 Cases," Pediatrics, Vol. 27, No. 2, February 1961. [18] Money, J., Hampson, J. G., and Hampson, J. L., "Imprinting and the Establishment of Gender Role," Archives Neurology and Psychiatry, Vol. 77,1957, p. 333.[19] Lukianowicz, D. P. M., "Survey of Various Aspects of Transvestism," J. Nervous & Mental Diseases, Vol. 128, No. 1, January 1959. [20] Prince, C. V., "166 Men in Dresses," Sexology Magazine, March 1962, pp. 520-525.
|
![]() |